- Cross-generational coalitions – A parent enlists a child to create a coalition against the other parent.
- A parent will get all of their emotional support in relation to spousal problems from one of the children.
Hyper-alignment with a parent, accompanied by rejection of the other, is seen in some children of divorce and separation. It is also seen in other family situations and can be seen across generations even when families live together. The usual scenario in which this pattern of hyper-alignment and rejection is seen in divorce and separation, is when the child hyper-aligns with the parent with whom they live with for most of the time, but it is not always this way. Some children who spend time with a parent that they do not live with, are also manipulated into the hyper-alignment and rejection pattern of behaviour. These patterns of family alignments and coalitions is well understood in the family therapy literature which describes the existence of behaviours in families which cause the risk of hyper-alignment between parent and child.
Enmeshment is when there is not a well-defined emotional boundary between two people in a family.
- A person in an enmeshed relationship will feel the emotions of the other person without having the ability to understand that it is not their emotion or that the emotion did not originate with them.
The key to understanding how this happens is not to look at the relationship with the parent the child is rejecting but the parent with whom the child is hyper aligned, because it is within this relationship that the answers to why this pattern of behaviour has arisen in the child is found. When this is understood by professionals, rapid progress can be made in protecting the child from harm.
Most cases of hyper-alignment between a parent and child, will come to light after family separation but the patterns of behaviours will have been in place in many families before the breakdown of the adult relationship. Hyper-alignment between parent and child, is caused by many things which are not readily seen by the outside world and in fact the problem can look like an extremely close relationship (my daughter is my best friend, my son is my rock), are good examples of how these relationships present themselves.
Hyper-alignment is caused by enmeshment, which is a diffusing of the boundaries between the parent and child, where the child is inveigled into the adult relationship to support a parent. Examples of this are when a parent has a weak ego (sense of self) and looks to a child to make them feel good about themselves, this removes from the child, their own sense of vulnerability and builds instead a false sense of self, in which the child believes that they are both responsible for and the saviour of, a parent who is not coping well. This leads to the onset of parentification an attachment distortion which causes life long psychological and emotional harm if it is not interrupted. It is not for children to give emotional care and stability to their parents, when this occurs, the life chances of the child are changed, sometimes irrevocably. It is in the hyper alignment with a parent, that the behaviours in the parent, which cause this pattern of behaviour in children are found.
Treating hyper-alignment in children of divorce, requires a depth understanding of the hyper-alignment relationships which are seen as the presenting problem, as well as an ability to provide stabilisation, regulation and resilience building in parents in the rejected position. Understanding how the child came to be in the hyper-aligned relationship, requires a close examination of the parent to whom the child is hyper-aligned as well as an evaluation of the capacity of the parent in the rejected position, to take up and use therapeutic parenting skills. Using a trauma informed approach to doing this work, the rejected parent is evaluated using strengths based assessments and is supported to stablise any reactive splitting by using co-regulation strategies between therapist and parent. Learning how reactive splitting* causes dysregulation when trying to engage with the child, helps parents in the rejected position to avoid the traps of negative projection which are strong in these systems. Working as a team, therapist and parent provide a co-regulated space in which the hyper-aligned child can experience the split off part of self in the form of the rejected parent. This is careful and delicate attachment focused work which heals the splitting in the child which causes hyper-alignment and its associated psychopathologies. This is the start of the child’s emergence from the hyper alignment, which is about extracting the child from the emotional and psychological double binds, which are woven in the inter-psychic relationship between influencing parent and child.
In structural terms there is a difference between the way in which fathers influence or manipulate their children to become hyper-aligned and the way in which mothers do this. Fathers are far more likely to use strategies which are visible and understood as coercive control. Children who are hyper-aligned with their fathers, are more likely to be verbally and physically aggressive to their mothers whilst children who are hyper-aligned with their mothers are more likely to be enmeshed, parentified and passive in their resistance to their fathers. The similarities in all children who reject are the presence of psychological splitting, which is seen on the outside as a hyper alignment with one and rejection of the other parent but which on the inside, is a splitting of the ego or sense of self in the child, which causes a false and omnipotent self which lacks empathy and is often contemptuous. This occurs whether a child is hyper-aligned with a father or a mother and it is this pattern which shows us that this is a distinct issue for children of divorce and separation which exists outside of feminist theories of coercive control.
Working with a psycho-neuro-biological understanding of what is happening to these children and a structural approach to therapeutic intervention, brings clarity to the treatment route and demonstrable success even in the most complex of cases. In 2023, the evaluation of the FSC approach will bring transparency this work through the voices of children who are now over the age of eighteen, who were hyper-aligned and as a result were found to have been emotionally and psychologically harmed by the family courts in the UK. These children were moved from fathers to mothers and mothers to fathers during the last decade and this evaluation is part of a year of outputs from FSC which will make this work transparent as well as replicable, in ways which demonstrate that the claims made by those who try to deny that children of divorce can and are caused serious emotional and psychological harm by parents who manipulate them, are quite simply, untrue.
*reactive splitting is a defensive process observed in many rejected parents, in which the psychological and emotional distress caused by a child’s contemptuous rejection causes splitting off of parts of self as well as reactive projections of hurt and anger. This can contribute to a feedback loop of splitting and projection which becomes entrenched.
2023 Resources for Parents
Listening and Learning Circles
These popular online circles are for all parents and grandparents and wider family members in the rejected position, who want to learn more about therapeutic parenting and how it helps alienated children. The only requirement for attendance is curiosity and a willingness to listen and learn and, where you feel you can, share your experience. Facilitated by me, the next six sessions are as follows –
January 24 – 19:00-21:00 GMT
Understanding Latent Vulnerability and How Therapeutic Parenting Helps
This is a circle which focuses upon the longer term needs of children who have suffered induced psychological splitting. Introducing the trauma concept of Latent Vulnerability, the skills to work with children who have suffered attachment disruption, which is seen to cause psychological splitting, will be explored.
Cost £40 – Family and friends can attend for the cost of one place.
Book Here
February 7 – 19:00-21:00 GMT
Supporting Grandparents to find their healing place and power
This circle is for grandparents in families where children align and reject. It is to enable grandparents to understand what is happening to children and how they are well placed to provide the help that children need. Based upon Structural Therapy, this circle will work with hierarchies in families and how to build healthy structures for children both absent, present and returning.
Cost £40 – Family and friends can attend for the cost of one place.
Book Here
February 21 – 19:00-21:00 GMT
Communicating with Alienated Children
Therapeutic Parenting skills can be used even if you are not able to see your children. By understanding the child’s experience, it is possible to find a way of communicating which can trigger change in your child’s responses to you. Based upon the successful circle held in 2022, ‘Writing to your Alienated Child’ which has produced a significant number of reports of successful reconnection following use of the strategies shared in that circle, this circle will expand upon communication strategies using creativity and curiosity as well as therapeutic parenting skills
Cost £40 – Family and friends can attend for the cost of one place.
Book Here
March 7 – 19:00 -21:00 GMT
Introduction to Therapeutic Parenting Skills
This is an introductory session for parents who are new to therapeutic parenting. Using basic skills as a starter, we will explore how understanding the self as a therapeutic parent, changes the way that you signal your position to your child. Whilst this is an introductory session, all parents are encouraged to join this circle to build up shared momentum for knowledge and skills amongst rejected parents. This develops the capacity of the rejected parent community to assist other parents who are new to this experience.
Cost £40 – Family and friends can attend for the cost of one place.
Book Here
March 21 – 19:00-21:00
Helping the Parentified Child
Parentification is one of the key problems facing children who are manipulated in divorce and separation, it is a covert manipulation which can be difficult to spot, precisely because, as Dr Steve Miller always pointed out, it looks like a close and loving relationship.
There is no need to be helpless in the face of the parentified child however and, because the relational networks in the brain are constantly open to change, learning how to help the parentified child is a powerful tool to have at the ready for any parent who has been forced into the rejected position.
This circle will focus upon understanding how parentified children behave and how to operationalise strategies to help them.
Cost £40 – Family and friends can attend for the cost of one place.
Book Here
April 4 – 19:00-21:00
What is really happening when a child rejects a parent outright
The evidence is clear that a child who rejects a parent outright after divorce and separation, is not doing so because that parent is abusive. Instead, it is the parent to whom the child is aligned who is causing harm and it is the alignment we should be looking at because it is this which is abusive to the child. It is abusive because, even though it looks like love, it is a fear based response which is underpinned by the biological imperative to survive. In the framework of latent vulnerability, what we are seeing when a child aligns in this way, is a child who is already vulnerable in the parental relationship, succumbing to underlying disorganised attachments. This circle will explore the reality of what happens when a child rejects a parent and will focus on how therapeutic parenting can assist the child to recover.
Cost £40 – Family and friends can attend for the cost of one place.
Book Here
Online Courses for Parents
Holding Up A Healthy Mirror
This popular Therapeutic Parenting Course will be available on demand shortly.
Higher Level Understanding
This live course for those who have completed HUAM either live with me in 2022 or on demand in 2023, will be delivered three times over the coming year, I will announce the next delivery shortly.
Trainings for Parent Coaches
I will deliver a training for coaches who wish to use therapeutic parenting with clients in the first half of 2023 and will announce delivery in the coming weeks here.
Trainings and Resources for Practitioners
We are in the process of developing a suite of trainings and resources for practitioners which will be delivered from a dedicated training platform, I will update when these are ready here.
Instructing the Family Separation Clinic in Court
We regret that we cannot accept any instructions from the lower courts in 2023. High Court instructions for clinical trials for therapy in cases where children have been found to have been emotionally and psychologically harmed can be accepted, please do not propose the Clinic without enquiring about our availability first. Please see here for enquiries
Coaching and Psychotherapy
We have limited space for coaching and psychotherapy, please enquire at appts@familyseparationclinic for availability
New for 2023
If you would like to be added to the mailing list for my newsletter for Therapeutic Parents, please mail me at parenting@familyseparationclinic.co.uk and head your email – ‘please add me’. The Newsletter goes out regularly and gives you full details of all online groups and resources as well as giving you reading lists and reminders to support your therapeutic parenting journey.


Defense mechanism is predicted by attachment and mediates the maladaptive influence of insecure attachment on adolescent mental health
Clarissa Laczkovics, Gregory Fonzo, Brianna Bendixsen, Emmanuel Shpigel, Ihno Lee, Katrin Skala, Antonio Prunas, James Gross, Hans Steiner & Julia Huemer
Current Psychology volume 39, pages1388–1396 (2020)Cite this article
Abstract
There is limited research exploring attachment style and defenses in adolescents. The purpose of the current research is to explore the relationship between adolescent attachment style and development of defense mechanisms, as well as attachment style and problem behaviors. A total of 1487 students from two California high-schools completed three self-report questionnaires to establish defense mechanisms, psychiatric symptoms, and attachment style. Attachment styles characterized by a positive self-image predict greater levels of mature defense mechanisms, and lower levels of immature defense mechanisms, both in the interpersonal and intrapsychic domains. Relationships between insecure attachment styles and psychopathology were mediated by greater levels of immature defense mechanisms. These results provide initial compelling evidence that: a) attachment style is an important determinant of the type of defense mechanisms utilized by the individual to maintain psychological stability; and b) defense mechanisms serve to transmit the detrimental effects of insecure attachment style on psychological health.
Introduction
Defense mechanisms function at an unconscious level to prevent conflicts and accompanied anxiety from entering awareness (Vaillant 1994). They work either to cope with conflicts of the “inner world” or may skew an individual’s perception of reality. Consequently, defenses function permanently to maintain psychological stability (Malone et al. 2013). A growing body of evidence demonstrates defense mechanism adaptivity impacts various measures of wellbeing, such as physical (Malone et al. 2013) and psychological outcomes (McMahon et al. 2005; Vaillant 2000).
A vast majority of the literature distinguishes between mature, neurotic and immature defense mechanisms, but there is an ongoing debate about how to best categorize and label them. Aligned with Vaillant’s original model of immature versus mature defenses, the REM-71 is a self-report instrument which has good psychometric properties (Araujo et al. 2006; Steiner et al. 2001) that organizes defenses into two categories: Factor 1 defenses (assimilation) and Factor 2 defenses (accommodation) (Prunas et al. 2014). Factor 1 and Factor 2 are each sub-categorized into intrapsychic defenses, which are internally focused, and interpersonal defenses, which manifest in the context of relating to others (Steiner et al. 2001). The terms “assimilation,” or absorbing external information based on one’s internal schema, and “accommodation,” or modifying internal schemas based on external information, represent a similar adaptive hierarchy as “immature” and “mature,” respectively. However, assimilation and accommodation are less ambiguous labels, as “maturity” can refer either to a developmental construct or adaptive efficacy. Using the REM-71, defense mechanisms have been associated with psychosocial functioning (Steiner et al. 2001) and psychiatric symptoms (Besharat and Khajavi 2013; Prunas et al. 2014) in adolescents, with accommodation promoting more adaptive functioning than assimilation.
Defenses have long been conceptualized as a developmental phenomenon that emerge early in infancy (Cramer 1991; Freud 1979). In newborns, “immature” defense mechanisms, like splitting or fantasy, are present, and they develop over early childhood to more mature ones. This development is thought to be influenced by the primary relationships. Therefore, one worthwhile area of study is the potential influence of attachment style (Besharat and Khajavi 2013). Attachment refers to the formation of interpersonal emotional bonds.
Attachment behavior is activated especially when the infant is experiencing fear, in strange situations, under pain or overwhelmed by its fantasies. The infant is hoping for a secure place near its primary caregiver. It is an active partner in this interaction. The empathic reaction in several interactions leads to the unique inner working model of the child (Bowlby 1973). In the beginning the working models are flexible, throughout development they lead to a stable representation of attachment being a behavioral system which is based on information processing.
It can be thought of as a system that regulates emotion through interpersonal dynamics, given that a child manages feelings of stress or fear through his proximity to a caregiver (Guttmann-Steinmetz and Crowell 2006). Attachment styles are quite stable (Ravitz et al. 2010). Bowlby described two prominent features of infant attachment that become one’s prototype for future relationships: an internal working model of the self as worthy or unworthy of love and support, and an internal working model of others as a reliable or unreliable source of love and support (Bowlby 1973). Bartholomew combined these internal working models of self-image and image of others to produce four theoretical adult attachment styles: secure, preoccupied, fearful, and dismissing (Bartholomew and Horowitz 1991).
Securely attached individuals, characterized by a positive self-image and a positive image of others, have both a sense of worthiness of love and an expectation that others are responsive. A preoccupied attachment style (negative self-image and positive image of others) results in a dependence on the acceptance of others for self-worth. The fearful style (negative self-image and negative image of others) leads to an avoidance of close relationships as a means to protect oneself against anticipated rejection. Finally, the dismissing (positive self-image and negative image of others) individual avoids close relationships as a way to prevent being disappointed by others. Preoccupied, fearful, and dismissing styles all indicate insecure attachment. Griffin and Bartholomew furthermore suggested that the self and other models are fundamental dimensions of attachment that underlie all measures of adult attachment (Griffin and Bartholomew 1994).
Although attachment styles and defensive mechanisms are distinct concepts of how we cope with stressful situations, both refer to a form of information processing. Defensive mechanisms are stable dynamic patterns, attachment style is a behavioral concept. Both are thought to develop in early childhood, and in recent literature, parallels between these systems are found. Ciocca et al. (Ciocca et al. 2017) showed that paranoid ideation characterized by immature defense mechanisms is clearly associated with an insecure attachment style. The same author (Ciocca et al. 2015) demonstrated that immature defense mechanisms and fearful attachment styles are involved in homophobic attitudes, whereas neurotic defense mechanisms and secure attachment are indicating low levels of homophobia.
Defense mechanisms of pregnant women predicted attachment security in their toddlers as mature, healthy defenses were significantly associated with greater toddler attachment security. Possible mechanisms discussed included parental attunement and mentalization. (Porcerelli et al. 2016)
A large body of research has shown that an individual’s attachment style influences a range of psychological and social factors (Ravitz et al. 2010), including externalizing behavior problems (Guttmann-Steinmetz and Crowell 2006), internalizing psychiatric symptoms (Esbjorn et al. 2012), and emotion regulation (Esbjorn et al. 2012; Gillath et al. 2005; Guttmann-Steinmetz and Crowell 2006; Moutsiana et al. 2014; Roque et al. 2013). Insecure maternal attachment was found to be associated with depression in ADHD children. (López Seco et al. 2016). Mothers´ arousal and regulation while parenting interacted to predict infant attachment disorganization and behavior problems (Leerkes et al. 2017). Also, the interaction of insecure attachment and behavioral inhibition increased the risk for internalizing problems such as anxiety (Lewis-Morrarty et al. 2015). Defense mechanisms, attachment styles and psychotic phenomena were studied in a non-clinical sample of students (Berry et al. 2006). The authors observed significant associations between insecure attachment and non-clinical psychotic phenomena.
Additionally, emerging research suggests one’s attachment style also impacts physical health and thus makes it even more worthwhile studying, also outside the scope of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.
Esposito et al. found a higher prevelance of avoidant attachment style and a significantly lower prevalence of the secure attachment style among children affected by migraine without aura compared with the normal controls. Another study (Davies et al. 2009) showed that patients with chronic widespread pain were more likely to report a preoccupied, dismissing or fearful attachment style than those free of pain. Morover, insecure attachment style was associated with the number of pain sites, the degree of pain related disability but not with pain intensity. Anno et al. (Anno et al. 2015) showed that in a general adult populationt the prevalence of chronic pain was higher in individuals reporting affectionless parental bonding style in childhood. Mc Williams et al. (McWilliams 2017) found that even after adjusting for depressive and anxiety disorders attachment insecurity was positively associated with medically unexplained chronic pain.
Migraine headaches (Esposito et al. 2013), chronic pain (Davies et al. 2009), chronic illness, and disease (McWilliams and Bailey 2010; McWilliams et al. 2010), have all been attributed to insecure attachment styles.
The findings of Adams et al. (Adams and McWilliams 2015) demonstrate that sleep disturbances independent of health conditions and concurrent psychiatric disorders is related to attachment insecurity. And also in eating disorders attachment was found to be a predictor of risk. (Szalai and Czegledi 2015); (Jewell et al. 2016)
However, there remains a paucity of research on the relationship between attachment and defenses. The few studies that have been conducted indicate an association between attachment styles and specific defense mechanisms. For example, more adaptive defenses are one well-validated indicator of resilience, found to relate to secure attachment (Simeon et al. 2007). In addition, maladaptive defenses have been associated with insecure attachment (Cramer and Kelly 2010; Lopez 2001; Mikulincer and Horesh 1999). Defense mechanisms have also been found to play a partially mediating role between insecure attachment and alexithymia in a group of adolescents (Besharat and Khajavi 2013). Still, empirical data is lacking on the connection between attachment styles and the development of defense mechanisms. Furthermore, no study has explored the relationship between defenses and working models of the self and other, despite the centrality of self and other working models to attachment (Griffin and Bartholomew 1994).
Here, we describe the first attempt to address these gaps in knowledge by investigating the relationship between attachment styles and 21 defense mechanisms as measured by the REM-71 in a non-clinical group of 1487 adolescents. We hypothesized that attachment style influences development of defenses. Given Griffin and Bartholomew’s hypothesis that self and other working models are fundamental attachment dimensions, we hypothesized that self- and other-models specifically influence the development of defenses. As such, we expected attachment styles with positive internal working models of self/others (secure) to be associated with adaptive defense mechanisms (accommodation), while attachment styles characterized by negative internal working models of self/others would be associated with maladaptive defense mechanisms (assimilation). In addition to this, we aimed to extend our insight into this association and build on previous research to more precisely explore the relationship between attachment style and problem behaviors (Erkan et al. 2015). In particular, we hypothesized that attachment would predict problem behaviors and that this relationship is mediated by defense mechanisms.
Methods
Participants
Subjects were students from two local suburban high schools in California, U.S., who agreed to participate in the study. Informed consent was given by the participating student. Informed consent was additionally given by the parents or legal guardian in case of the age being below 16 years to participate in the study, which was approved by the institutional review board of Stanford University. Clinically trained individuals administered the questionnaires to subjects after they had been given sufficient information regarding the purpose of the study. The study was carried out in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Measures
Defense Mechanisms
To evaluate defense mechanisms, participants completed the Response Evaluation Measure (REM-71) (Steiner and Feldman 1995), a 71-item, self-report questionnaire that has been validated for the assessment of 21 defenses in child, adolescent, and adult populations (Araujo et al. 2006; Steiner et al. 2001; Yasnovsky et al. 2003). Each defense is assessed based on the average score of the three or four items that represent that defense. Each item is rated on a 9-point scale from “strongly disagree” (“1”) to “strongly agree” (“9”). The current study utilized a two-factor solution yielded by un-rotated principal components analysis. This has been confirmed in a second large sample of Italian adolescents and adults (Prunas et al. 2009). Factor 1 (F1) is comprised of 14 assimilation defense mechanisms that distort reality according to expected outcome. Factor 2 (F2) is comprised of 7 accommodation defense mechanisms that attenuate distressing reality. Factor 1 and Factor 2 defenses are further differentiated into intrapsychic and interpersonal defenses.
Psychiatric Symptoms
To assess psychiatric symptoms, subjects completed the Youth Self-Report (YSR) (Achenbach and Rescorla 2001). This self-report measure is designed to assess emotional and behavioral problems in adolescents. Subjects endorse 112 items on a three-point Likert scale from “not true” (“0”), “somewhat or sometimes true” (“1”), to “very true or often true” (“2”). The YSR yields scores for eight empirically derived narrow-band syndrome scales: social problems, withdrawal, anxiety/depression, somatic complaints, thought problems, attention problems, delinquency, and aggression. These scales are further grouped into two broad-band summary syndromes: Externalizing (aggressive, gets into fights, undercontrolled) and Internalizing (depressed, withdrawn, overcontrolled). A Total Problem Behaviors score is also provided. The YSR scales have well-established validity and reliability.
Attachment Style
To evaluate attachment style, subjects completed the Relationship Scales Questionnaire (RSQ), a 30-item self-report dimensional measure of attachment (Griffin and Bartholomew 1994) that yields scores for Bartholomew and Horowitz’s four attachment styles (secure, dismissing, fearful, and preoccupied) (Bartholomew and Horowitz 1991). The measure uses a 5-point Likert-type scale for each item ranging from “not at all like me” to “very much like me”. The RSQ has demonstrated good reliability and convergent validity (Bartholomew and Horowitz 1991), and longitudinal studies have shown the RSQ to have temporal stability (Scharfe and Bartholomew 1994; Scharfe and Cole 2006).
Continuous scores of each of the four attachment styles are derived by calculating the mean rating of the items representing each attachment prototype. We used these continuous variables to calculate path analyses. As suggested by Griffin and Bartholomew, self-model scores and other-model scores, which have demonstrated construct validity and can be reliably assessed by self-report measures, were derived from the four attachment style scores (Griffin and Bartholomew 1994). As such, the self-model score was obtained by summing the ratings of the two attachment domains with positive self-model (secure and dismissing) and subtracting the ratings of the two attachment domains with negative self- model (preoccupied and fearful) (Griffin and Bartholomew 1994). The other-model score was obtained by summing the ratings of the two attachment domains with positive other-model (secure and preoccupied) and subtracting the ratings of the two attachment domains with negative other-model (dismissing and fearful) (Griffin and Bartholomew 1994).
Analyses
Since our sample was of considerable size, we decided to perform a split sample algorithm carried out in R, which allowed us to create two data sets, one for exploratory, the other one for confirmatory analyses. Carrying out a confirmatory analysis yields a clear formulation of a theory to be tested in its application and thus reaches more generalizable results.
Statistics for both exploratory and confirmatory analyses were first calculated using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 21. We performed linear regressions with sex, age, and self-model score/other-model score as independent variables, and intrapsychic and interpersonal assimilation/accomodation as dependent variables.
In addition, path analysis, a method to determine whether or not a multivariate set of non-experimental data fits well with a particular (a priori) causal model, was employed and carried out in R. Four focal predictors (continuous variables of attachment styles), 4 mediators (interpersonal/intrapsychic assimilation/accommodation), 2 outcomes (internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors), and 2 covariates (age, sex) of all 10 variables were added to the path analysis. Again, path analyses were performed for each exploratory and confirmatory data set separately.
Results
Sample
Of the students surveyed, 89% returned valid surveys. Of these 1487 students, 45% were male (N = 663) and their mean age was 15.9 years (S.D. = 1.2, range 13–20). Ethnic composition of the sample was as follows: Caucasian 39.5%, Hispanic 25.8%, Asian 12.9%, Other 11.8%. Their parents’ employment levels were average for the region (94% of fathers and 82% of mothers were employed; 77% of the students came from two-income homes).
Attachment and Problem Behaviors, Mediated by Defense Mechanisms
We used path analyses to test the hypothesis that attachment would predict problem behaviors as assessed with the Youth Self Report, which would be mediated by defense mechanisms.
Four focal predictors (continuous variables of attachment styles), 4 mediators (interpersonal/intrapsychic assimilation/accommodation), 2 outcomes (internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors), and 2 covariates (age, sex) of all 10 variables were added to the path analyses.
Due to the large number of indirect effects we were testing, we report here only those significant at p < 0.01. Internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors were predicted by both preoccupied attachment (p < 0.01) and fearful attachment (p < 0.01), showing a significant positive relationship between problem behaviors and attachment domains. These relationships were significantly mediated both by intrapsychic and interpersonal assimilation. Internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors were also predicted by the secure attachment domain, showing a significant negative relationship (p < 0.01), that was mediated by interpersonal assimilation.
Analyses in the Confirmatory Dataset
Self Versus Other Model: Independent Contributions of Self Versus Other Model to Interpersonal and Intrapsychic Assimilation and Accommodation
All of the described significant relationships from the exploratory dataset could be replicated in the confirmatory dataset, with the self-model score contributing significantly to the regression model, both for the intrapsychic as well as the interpersonal domain of assimilation and accommodation. The same set of relationships could be established for the other-model score, with the exception, that, like in the exploratory dataset, there was no significant contribution of the other-model score and the intrapsychic domain of accommodation.
Attachment and Problem Behaviors, Mediated by Defense Mechanisms
The most significant effects from the exploratory dataset were replicated in the confirmatory sample. In addition to replicating the findings from the exploratory dataset, the mentioned relationship for externalizing problem behavior was mediated by interpersonal assimilation for the preoccupied and fearful attachment domain, and by interpersonal accommodation only for the secure attachment domain (Tables 3 and 4).
Discussion
To our knowledge, this study presents the first pieces of evidence that attachment style influences the predominant expression of defense mechanisms, and these defense mechanisms serve to mediate the association between insecure attachment styles and psychopathology. We utilized a large sample of healthy adolescents to derive these findings and furthermore replicated effects in a separate sample. This study produced the following primary findings. Attachment styles characterized by a positive self-image predict greater levels of mature defense mechanisms, those characterized by accommodation, and lower levels of immature defense mechanisms, those characterized by assimilation, both in the interpersonal and intrapsychic domains. Attachment styles characterized by a positive other self-image predict lower levels of assimilation in the interpersonal and intrapsychic domains, but higher levels of accommodation in only the interpersonal domain. Relationships between several insecure attachment styles and psychopathology were mediated by greater levels of assimilation and lower levels of accommodation. In aggregate, these results provide initial compelling evidence that: a) the attachment style lets us deduce to the type of defense mechanisms utilized by the individual to maintain psychological stability; and b) defense mechanisms serve to transmit the detrimental effects of insecure attachment style on psychological health.
Our results are generally in line with research that links mature defenses, i.e. accommodation, with secure attachment (Simeon et al. 2007) and immature defenses, i.e. assimilation, with insecure attachment (Cramer and Kelly 2010; Lopez 2001; Mikulincer and Horesh 1999). In a recent study using the Adult Attachment Interview, findings suggest that adult attachment in patients with systemic lupus erythematosus has an influence on the presence of alexithymic features (Barbasio and Granieri 2013). We postulate that emotion regulation difficulties associated with insecure attachment may inhibit the development of adaptive defense mechanisms, while secure attachment may facilitate the use of more mature defense mechanisms (Besharat and Khajavi 2013). It is also possible that insecure attachment is a developmental risk factor for consolidating maladaptive defense mechanisms, which convey the influence of attachment style on emotion regulation efficacy (Besharat and Khajavi 2013).
Here, we expand previous research by differentiating between intrapsychic and interpersonal defenses. Our results demonstrate striking connections between internal working models of the self and intrapsychic defense mechanisms, and internal working models of others and interpersonal defense mechanisms. We found attachment styles characterized by a negative self-image to be associated with intrapsychic assimilation. In other words, the view that one is unworthy of love and support was linked with internally focused maladaptive defense mechanisms. Likewise, attachment styles with positive self-image were significantly associated with intrapsychic accommodation, attachment characterized by a sense of worthiness of love and support was linked with internally focused adaptive defenses. This pattern was also found for interpersonal defenses: interpersonal assimilation was significantly associated with attachment styles characterized by a negative image of others; while interpersonal accommodation associated with attachment styles characterized by a positive view of others.
While some caution should be exercised in interpreting these associations due to the fundamental differences in the constructs of attachment and defenses (Ravitz et al. 2010), there does therefore seem to be a parallel between one’s internal working models and maturity of defenses.
Results of our path analysis indicate that assimilation (interpersonal and intrapsychic) indirectly affect the relationship between attachment styles characterized by a negative view of the self and internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors. In other words, assimilation may build a bridge between a negative internal working model of the self and problem behaviors. This is in line with research indicating that insecure attachment may put children on a path toward developing problem behaviors or psychiatric symptoms (Ciocca et al. 2017; Esbjorn et al. 2012; Guttmann-Steinmetz and Crowell 2006).
By mapping an attachment model differentiated by self-image and image of others onto the adaptiveness of intrapsychic and interpersonal defense mechanisms, our study supports the hypothesis that facets of attachment exert influences on the development of defense mechanisms—put differently, the way in which one relates to others early in life may affect how maturely developed the defense mechanisms used for self-regulation are. (one then self-regulates using defense mechanisms.) The parallels between the four attachment styles and defenses found in our study also support the organization of defense mechanisms into assimilation and accommodation categories as summarized by Factor 1 and Factor 2 of the REM-71. Finally, our results highlight the value of differentiating between intrapsychic and interpersonal defense mechanisms when assessing their relationship with other psychological constructs.
Limitations of this study include the retrospective nature of the Bartholomew Attachment Interview and the use of self-report instruments, which can allow for biased responses due to either the desire for social desirability or the pre- and unconscious level of information processing. The cross-sectional study design also prevents us from being able to draw causal conclusions; longitudinal studies are needed in order to establish causal relationships between attachment and defenses. Research on large samples of clinical populations of adolescents would be beneficial in order to compare results with nonclinical samples. In addition, further research is required in order to better understand the influential role that attachment styles may have on defenses, to evaluate the predictive value of attachment with regard to defenses, and to explore additional variables that may influence how defenses originate and evolve.
Given our findings that attachment has a significant effect on defenses, as well as the growing body of literature suggesting both attachment and defenses have a significant impact on physical health (Davies et al. 2009; Esposito et al. 2013; McWilliams and Bailey 2010; McWilliams et al. 2010), it is important to consider the medical implications of these factors when assessing a pediatric patient.
Summary
In conclusion, these results provide initial compelling evidence that: a) attachment style is an important determinant of the type of defense mechanisms utilized predominantly by the individual to maintain psychological stability; and b) defense mechanisms serve to transmit the detrimental effects of insecure attachment style on psychological health.
https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12144-018-9839-1
SO, THE IMPACT OF THE DEFENSE MECHANISM IS DESTRUCTIVE FOR (PARTNER & CHILDREN) EVERYBODY.
Defense mechanism is predicted by attachment and mediates the maladaptive influence of insecure attachment on adolescent mental health
Discussion
[1] Our results are generally in line with research that links mature defenses, i.e. accommodation, with secure attachment (Simeon et al. 2007) and immature defenses, i.e. assimilation, with insecure attachment (Cramer and Kelly 2010; Lopez 2001; Mikulincer and Horesh 1999).
[2] In a recent study using the Adult Attachment Interview, findings suggest that adult attachment in patients with systemic lupus erythematosus has an influence on the presence of alexithymic features (Barbasio and Granieri 2013).
Summary
In conclusion, these results provide initial compelling evidence that:
[3] a) attachment style is an important determinant of the type of defense mechanisms utilized predominantly by the individual to maintain psychological stability; and
[4] b) defense mechanisms serve to transmit the detrimental effects of insecure attachment style on psychological health.
I HAVE READ A BOOK WHERE IT IS EXPLAINED IN CONTENT WHAT IS THE PURPOSE OF THE OFFENDER BUT ALSO THE EXPERIENCE OF THE VICTIM WITH THE PSYCHICAL INQUIRIES THAT THE VICTIM DEVELOPS.
Passive-Aggression: Understanding the Sufferer, Helping the Victim, 2nd Edition (Martin Kantor) https://www.amazon.nl/-/en/Martin-Kantor-ebook/dp/B075RTPHJW
SO THE BEHAVIOR OF THE ALIENATED PARENT, AND ALIENATED CHILD ARE UNDERSTANDABLE BECAUSE THE BEHAVIOR OF THE ALIENATING
(PASSIVE-AGGRESSIVE) PARENT IS ALSO DESCRIBED.
AND THE GAP LEVEL 1. (passive-aggression) LEVEL 0. (psychotic) IS SMALLER THAN
THE GAP LEVEL 4. (immature defense reaches to level 1) TO MATURE DEFENSE LEVEL. 7
Psychoanalysis of the Psychoses: Current Developments in Theory and Practice
(Edited by Riccardo Lombardi, Luigi Rinaldi and Sarantis Thanopulos)
Chapter 7 – The psychoanalyst and psychosis: The bull in a China shop (Stefano Calamandrei)
Psychotic mental functioning
It must be considered that, in relational situations, which are often of high emotional intensity for those who, like psychotics, are psychologically fragile, it is a common characteristic of these patients to be frightened by what is happening to them since they realise that they are not in control of [window of tolerance faux] such powerful defence mechanisms. Indeed, when these defensive modes are put into action, they function intensely and automatically, and create more emotional difficulties than they solve. The patient has the wholly reasonable sensation that she is not the protagonist of her own psychic life.
This particular “feeling” which arises in the therapeutic encounter with a psychotic person is essentially due to the fact that another intense mechanism is being set in motion: the “avoidance” of any relationship. In the psychoanalytic session this difficulty acquires a determining significance, since such a lack of relationality is perceived by the therapist as the setting up of a particular distance by the patient.
This necessary form of withdrawal from others depends on the fact of having to reduce and avoid everything that stimulates her psychic life since the emotions it evokes reveal its existence, and the suffering becomes unbearable.
Every time there is an encounter with another person, there is the possibility of being struck by an intense emotional stimulation on both narcissistic and libidinal levels, and of undergoing an excessive release of mental sensations. The only defence that ensures self-preservation is the avoidance of the stimulating relationship.
Nurturing Resilience: Helping Clients Move Forward from Developmental Trauma
Kathy L. Kain and Stephen J. Terrell
The Faux Window of Tolerance
We can extend the Window of Tolerance model to include the understanding of how clients utilize various strategies for managing their dysregulation and uncontained responses when they leave the Window of Tolerance and enter into hyper-or hypo-arousal states, particularly if they are chronically unable to access the Optimal Arousal Zone (the ventral parasympathetic and low-tone dorsal physiology). In such cases, defensive accommodations develop to manage chronic levels of hyper-and hypo-arousal states. On top of the Window of Tolerance model, we can overlay what we have come to call the Faux Window of Tolerance. The model of the Faux Window provides a representation of what occurs when someone is chronically outside of his Window of Tolerance, and has developed defensive accommodations that effectively provide him with the experience of being within his Optimal Arousal Zone—when, in fact, he is operating chronically outside of that zone.
In this representation, in figure 11, the Window of Tolerance is shown as being narrowed, making it very challenging for the client to stay within that window once any sort of stimulus appears. This is what typically occurs in the face of developmental trauma. The chronic dysregulation that commonly accompanies early trauma usually forces this narrowing of the Optimal Arousal Zone to the point that almost anything bringing challenge to the system will push someone over the threshold of the window. In figure 11, we can see how this would occur with chronic hyper-arousal.
The Window of Tolerance is at the bottom of the diagram. On the hyperarousal side of the Window of Tolerance, we have indicated an additional window, which we term the Faux, or artificial, Window. In this range, the client manages her hyper-arousal states with defensive accommodations—selfsoothing behaviors like dissociation or compulsive eating—that hold her responses within a range that she experiences as manageable. The movement in and out of the Faux Window will occur much as it does within the Window of Tolerance, but the client may not actually reenter the Window of Tolerance, only stabilizing as best she can within a hyper-arousal state that is “workable” or “manageable” for tolerating her sympathetic activation symptoms and responses.
As with the Window of Tolerance, there can be a Faux Window on the hypoarousal side of the true Window of Tolerance. In this case, the client will manage his hypo-arousal states with defensive accommodations that would substitute for either the ventral parasympathetic system (social engagement) or the sympathetic (active response) system—perhaps by using stimulants, acting out, or entering hyper-sexuality—to keep his experience of the low-energy states of the dorsal physiology within a range that feels tolerable. Again, the defensive accommodations may not be sufficient to move the client back within the Window of Tolerance, instead stabilizing as much as possible within the collapsed, disconnected dorsal physiology. Figure 12 shows this version of the Faux Window.
AND THE BEHAVIOR NEVER STOPS,
BECAUSE THE SAME BEHAVIOR IS EVEN A HEAVY BURDEN FOR ANY THERAPIST!
FOR A CHILD….. DAY IN DAY OUT FOR YEARS ON YEARS ON NONSTOP ROCKING BEHIND CLOSED DOORS.
IF THE THERAPIST CAN'T HANDLE THOSE SITUATIONS, THAT MAKES RECOGNITION OF THOSE INTERACTION PATTERNS A NUMBER-ONE PRIORITY.
LikeLike
this is so helpful Bob. I am in a cabin in Crete, off grid for much of the time – recovering health and balance – back next week to carry on, am building this into my routine now, it is very necessary. Thank you for all of this. K
LikeLike